ALONE WITH YOUR THOUGHTS||
Imagine
you’re traveling in a bus, waiting in line or simply sitting on your
sofa at home, and you find yourself with a few minutes of idle time on your
hands – how might you occupy yourself? As most do nowadays, you’re likely to
start scrolling through your phone; but what if, instead, you chose to think
and entertain yourself with pleasurable thoughts? How hard could it be?
At first, it seems eminently
doable. After all, as humans, we possess remarkable minds that can reflect on
the past, process the present, and even make forecasts about the future. We can
project our thoughts on real or imagined, as well as pleasant or unpleasant,
scenarios and events. Given the endless forms our thoughts can take, why
wouldn’t we – when given the opportunity to withdraw from the external world –
dive into our own minds and entertain ourselves with our thoughts?
About 10 years ago, psychologists at the University of
Virginia and Harvard University began exploring this question by inviting
volunteers to spend time in a room void of all distractions (except for an
electric shock device) and to occupy themselves with their thoughts. Their
findings, published as a series of studies in
2014, were shocking: approx. 67 per cent
of the men and 25 per cent of the women
opted to intentionally shock themselves, rather than to spend a short amount of
time alone with their thoughts.
These findings seem surprising
for a species that’s defined by its unique thinking abilities .But it is important
to note that our brains have primarily evolved to regulate our body’s metabolic
needs through action and, while we do possess remarkable capacity to simulate
other worlds within our own minds, to do so is a cognitively demanding task and
one for which we’re not necessarily well practised. Perhaps another factor at
play, beyond the aversiveness of the effort involved, is that the benefit of
thinking pleasurable thoughts is not intuitive, especially considered against
the many competing things to think about – planning, problem-solving or even
ruminating over events. While attending to negative issues within our lives has
its uses, it certainly isn’t enjoyable. Thinking for the purpose of enjoyment
might therefore require practice and sufficient motivation.
To better understand why thinking
for pleasure is so challenging, it is helpful to break the activity into two
basic parts: the thought processes and the thought
contents. The thought processes refers to the intention (ie,
having sufficient motivation) and ability (ie, having sufficient concentration)
to think for pleasure, while the thought contents refers to
the subject matter of one’s thoughts, and whether they are sufficiently
meaningful and/or pleasurable.
Although people might generally
prefer external stimulation compared with thinking for pleasure, another recent
series of studies showed that thinking can be rendered more
enjoyable simply by boosting people’s intentions to use their thoughts in that
way – when researchers instructed participants to specifically think for
pleasure, rather than giving them the freedom to think about whatever they
wanted, the participants found the experience to be significantly more
enjoyable. This suggests that being motivated to think for pleasure, and
intentionally doing so, increases the enjoyment and does not merely occur to
people as a natural insight.
Turning to the second component
of the process side of thinking for pleasure – ie, having the ability and
concentration to generate the thoughts and to sustain one’s attention on such
thoughts for a prolonged period of time – another series of studies confirmed
the relevance of these factors. Following a period of trying to think for
pleasure, participants reported finding it particularly hard to concentrate
and, to the extent that they found it challenging, it significantly decreased
their reported enjoyment. Fortunately, the same research found an easy fix for
this aspect of the thinking process – when the researchers asked the
participants to first write down a list of enjoyable topics, and when they
could look at their list during the thinking break compared with not having
their list with them, the participants reported less difficulty in
concentrating, which led to greater overall enjoyment of the thinking time.
This brings us to the other key
part of thinking for pleasure – the content of one’s thoughts. What type of
thoughts provides the greatest enjoyment? And do we know what those thoughts
are? A recent research, suggests that most of us don’t know what to focus on
to generate the most enjoyment out of intentionally thinking for pleasure. But
some found that providing participants with examples of enjoyable thinking
topics (ie, accomplishments, social occasions, vacations) increased their
enjoyment, compared with having participants generate their own topics,
suggesting that they’d struggled to come up with these topics for themselves.
An intriguing pattern was found–whether directing
people to focus on meaningful thoughts would provide them with greater
enjoyment, rather than focusing on simply pleasurable ones. Researcher tested
this in a second study, by providing the same example topics to two groups of
participants, but they asked one to specifically think for meaning, while
another to think for pleasure. They found the thinkers were not enjoying it and
it was no more meaningful for them. So, if the people want to enjoy the
pleasurable thoughts they surely chose the topics which are not related to any
meaningful act.
Overall, findings demonstrate that, when people are asked
to think enjoyable thoughts, they don’t necessarily think of meaningful ones,
thus detracting from their pleasure, and when they are asked to think about
meaningful thoughts, they don’t necessarily think of enjoyable ones, again
undermining their pleasure. All of this suggests that most of us do not have an
intuitive sense of how and what to focus on in order to derive the most
pleasure from our thinking experience.
Fortunately, these results point to a couple of
scientifically backed ways for us to make thinking for pleasure actually
enjoyable. So, now that you are coming to the end of this article and you have
a few minutes to spare, why not spend some time with your own thoughts? Here’s
what I can recommend for optimising your enjoyment:
- Your goal should be to have a good time; that
is, make it your intent to focus on events and aspects of those events
that bring you enjoyment.
- When conjuring topics, choose ones that
are both meaningful and pleasurable (eg,
social events and accomplishments).
- Prior to the thinking period, write down these
topics, so that you can glance at them if you feel like you’re having a
hard time staying focused.
- Set out a specific time for your thinking
breaks; that is, choose to engage in the activity when you feel motivated
to think for pleasure, but stop if and when you feel like it’s becoming
too cognitively demanding.
- As with all things, practice – you will get
better over time and, the more you do it, the more you will anticipate
thinking for pleasure to be enjoyable.
The thoughts we create shape the
lives we live, hence it will be beneficial to occasionally spend some time
thinking for pleasure. While it is initially challenging, with a little
practice, we have at our abilities to derive pleasure from simply entertaining
our own thoughts. In a world in which the external environment demands so much
of our attention, I believe we could all benefit from occasionally retreating
inwards to derive both meaning and pleasure from within.
0 Comments
If you have any doubts, please let me know.